New Brain Tech Reveals Past Thoughts and Memories
An alarming new study (posted
in full below) illustrates how fast neuroscience is developing in its
attempt to uncover every aspect of the human brain. A specific brain
wave called P300 has been identified by researchers as a marker that
essentially encodes what we observe as we go about our daily activities.
Based on this specific brain wave marker, researchers are able to
conduct a Concealed Information Test. In fact, the test is already being
used in Japan and Israel. Researchers are hoping that new data they
have published will demonstrate that the test is reliable enough to meet
the higher standard of U.S. courtrooms.
The massive influx of money into Obama’s BRAIN project, as well as similar research sponsored by the European Union ,
now exceeds $2 billion combined. Research continues full-steam ahead
despite indications that human brain study is outpacing ethical
parameters. Some scientists within the European arm of the project have
recently threatened a boycott due to mismanagement and misuse along similar lines to what you will read below.
In our age of loosened constitutional
standards and basic human rights that has permitted all innocent
communications and movements to be tracked, traced and databased, any
technology that aims to uncover our most mundane daily activities and
thoughts must be heavily scrutinized for potential abuse. Moreover, I
have highlighted some of the language in this press release that clearly
demonstrates how this could move far beyond the courtroom and easily
provide a new level of pervasive surveillance.
Bold emphasis is mine:
Brain activity can be used to tell whether someone recognizes details they encountered in normal, daily life, which may have implications for criminal investigations and use in courtrooms, new research shows.
The findings , published in Psychological Science , a journal of the Association for Psychological Science , suggest that a particular brain wave, known as P300 , could serve as a marker that identifies places, objects, or other details that a person has seen and recognizes from everyday life .
Research using EEG recordings of brain
activity has shown that the P300 brain wave tends to be large when a
person recognizes a meaningful item among a list of nonmeaningful items.
Using P300, researchers can give a subject a test called the Concealed Information Test (CIT) to try to determine whether they recognize information that is related to a crime or other event.
Most studies investigating P300 and
recognition have been conducted in lab settings that are far removed
from the kinds of information a real witness or suspect might be exposed
to. This new study marks an important advance , says lead research John B. Meixner of Northwestern University, because it draws on details from activities in participants’ normal, daily lives.
“Much like a real crime, our participants
made their own decisions and were exposed to all of the distracting
information in the world,” he explains.
“Perhaps the most surprising finding was the extent to which we could detect very trivial details from a subject’s day, such as the color of umbrella that the participant had used,” says Meixner. “This
precision is exciting for the future because it indicates that
relatively peripheral crime details, such as physical features of the
crime scene, might be usable in a real-world CIT — though we still need
to do much more work to learn about this.”
To achieve a more realistic CIT, Meixner and
co-author J. Peter Rosenfeld outfitted 24 college student participants
with small cameras that recorded both video and sound — the students
wore the cameras clipped to their clothes for 4 hours as they went about
their day.
For half of the students, the researchers used the recordings to identify details specific to each person’s day, which became “probe” items for that person.
The researchers also came up with corresponding, “irrelevant”
items that the student had not encountered — if the probe item was a
specific grocery store, for example, the irrelevant items might include
other grocery stores.
For the other half of the students, the “probe”
items related to details or items they had not encountered, but which
were instead drawn from the recordings of other participants. The
researchers wanted to simulate a real investigation, in which a suspect
with knowledge of a crime would be shown the same crime-related details
as a suspect who may have no crime-related knowledge.
The next day, all of the students returned to
the lab and were shown a series of words that described different
details or items (i.e., the probe and irrelevant items), while their
brain activity was recorded via EEG.
The results showed that the P300 was larger
for probe items than for irrelevant items, but only for the students who
had actually seen or encountered the probe.
Further analyses revealed that P300
responses effectively distinguished probe items from irrelevant items on
the level of each individual participant, suggesting that it is a
robust and reliable marker of recognition.
These findings have implications for memory research, but they may also have real-world application in the domain of criminal law given that some countries, like Japan and Israel, use the CIT in criminal investigations.
“One reason that the CIT has not been
used in the US is that the test may not meet the criteria to be
admissible in a courtroom,” says Meixner. “Our work may help
move the P300-based CIT one step closer to admissibility by
demonstrating the test’s validity and reliability in a more realistic
context.”
Meixner, Rosenfeld, and colleagues plan on
investigating additional factors that may impact detection, including
whether images from the recordings may be even more effective at
eliciting recognition than descriptive words – preliminary data suggest
this may be the case.
Credits: Nicholas West, Guest
This article was originally featured at Activist Post.**
Aucun commentaire:
Enregistrer un commentaire